Note: the following has
been abstracted from the Grolier Encyclopedia.
Psychology
Psychology is
the systematic study of human and animal behavior. Psychologists try
to understand why living beings act the way they do, how they grow
up, how they learn and change, how they differ from one another, and
even how they get into trouble or become disturbed. Unlike PSYCHIATRY,
which is a medical specialty devoted to the understanding and cure
of mental disease, psychology has a broader task, ranging from the
laboratory study of simple behavior in animals (insects, worms, rats,
and pigeons have been commonly used in psychological experiments)
to the complicated behavior of human beings in social groups.
To be sure, some psychologists--clinical psychologists--devote most
of their efforts to helping disturbed, troubled, and mentally ill
people; clinical psychologists often use techniques much like those
employed by psychiatrists. Psychology is, however, far more than a
set of therapies for the troubled.
In a sense, psychology can be best understood as a federation of interests:
an alliance of scholars, scientists, and practitioners that is held
together by a shared commitment to the systematic study of human and
animal behavior. Psychologists believe that animal movements and human
minds can be examined as carefully and scientifically as astrophysicists
study galaxies or as biochemists study nucleic acids. No place exists
in modern psychology for phrenology, astrology, or other superstitions
about the causes of behavior.
About a century ago psychology became an independent field of study.
With its roots in philosophy, physiology, and psychiatry, the relatively
new field of psychology has maintained its breadth. It stretches from
biological study at one border, through the examination of human beings
in groups--which has much in common with sociology and anthropology--out
to the edges of clinical psychiatry and general medicine.
Several major dimensions define the ways in which contemporary psychologists
differ. One is the animal-human dimension. Some psychologists focus
on animals. The study of animals permits better isolation of important
causes of behavior and better experimental control. Most psychologists,
however, study some aspect of human behavior.
"One-several-many" is another dimension. Psychologists can
work with one person (for example, a neurotic in therapy), several
people (babies and parents to study emotional communication between
them, for instance), or large groups of people (such as the study
of mobs or cross-cultural comparisons).
The "species-individual" dimension also separates psychologists.
Many investigate what makes each individual unique; other psychologists
want to discover the characteristics of people (or animals) as members
of a particular species.
Finally, the "basic-applied" distinction, although not a
sharp one, refers to the tendency to concentrate either on basic research
and teaching in colleges and universities or on applied work in schools,
businesses, and clinics. About half of contemporary psychologists
are in basic work and half in applied. Interaction can be seen in
all fields. For the sake of economy and clarity, however, basic and
applied psychology are separated here to give an overview of contemporary
psychology.
FIELDS OF BASIC PSYCHOLOGY
Psychology as a Biological Science
Psychology as a Biological Science. Perhaps the oldest and longest
thread in the growth of psychology as a research discipline ties psychology
to biological study. Physiological and comparative psychology grew
primarily from the evolutionary theories of Charles Darwin; learning
was added as a major component of psychology near the beginning of
the 20th century. These three fields are discussed below.
Physiological Psychology
Applying neuroanatomy and neurophysiology to the study of behavior,
physiological psychology asks such questions as: Which structures
of the brain and nerves grow and change in animal and human action?
What are the neural networks that manage sensory experience? More
recently, physiological psychologists have become especially interested
in hormonal and biochemical changes in nerves, glands, and muscles--changes
that may be closely related to human development, emotion, and learning.
Much additional research is necessary before explicit links can be
made between DNA and learning French, for example, but physiological
psychologists are moving steadily toward a resolution of the ancient
question of how the brain relates to behavior.
Comparatie Psychology
Comparative psychology investigates animal behavior. More modest about
the lessons of animals for human beings than their 19th-century forerunners,
comparative psychologists continue to be interested in animal behavior
in its own right and as a potential model for understanding human
behavior. Ethology, a subgroup of comparative psychology, studies
animals ranging from ducks and rats to baboons, in natural settings,
and tries to draw general conclusions about patterns of mating, parental
care, and adaptation.
Learning
The study of learning is one of the central themes of psychology,
with connections to child psychology, physiological psychology, education,
and therapy. Learning psychologists examine simple behavior in animals--for
example, pigeons learning which of several keys to peck for a reward.
Learning psychologists also study human learning, design procedures
and devices for educational application (such as TEACHING MACHINES),
and develop programs for modifying problem behavior. In the history
of psychology, learning psychologists--especially Ivan PAVLOV, John
B. WATSON, Clark HULL, and B. F. SKINNER--have uniformly represented
psychology as a science, subject to the same rules of evidence and
inference that characterize physics or biology. They have also usually
argued for simple and straightforward explanations of behavior and
for the efficacy of reward and punishment in the modification of behavior.
Cognitive Psychology
Among the first issues that excited the interest of psychologists
in the early decades of the discipline were perception, memory, and
language. How do people come to interpret sights and sounds as meaningful
objects and events? How do people store evidence about what has happened
in order to remember the past? How is speech acquired? These questions
are still alive in psychology, and, together with inquiries about
imagery and thinking, they make up the broad field called cognitive
psychology.
Perception. Studies in perception investigate visual illusions,
the recognition of depth and color, and, increasingly, the ways people
put information together to make sense out of what they experience.
Adult humans are able to move through a rich and complicated world
of sensations and impressions; perceptual psychology examines how
people group, categorize, and organize all the evidence their senses
deliver.
Memory. The workings of the memory are akin to both perception
and thinking. Recent research has demonstrated that people have several
systems of memory: a short-term memory that holds images of sight
and sound just long enough for people to see properly and to hear;
a slightly longer-term memory that permits, for example, the storage
of telephone numbers long enough to get them dialed; and a long-term
memory that seems to hold the past almost indefinitely. Psychologists
who study memory tackle the mechanisms of memory encoding and retrieval
in all three forms.
Psycholinguistics. A significant segment of cognitive psychology,
psycholinguistics stretches from formal linguistics to social psychology.
Thinking. Cognitive psychologists study thinking from several
points of view. Some investigators carry out experiments on problem
solving, the making of inferences, and the use of analogies in thought;
others examine real-life thought in games such as chess or in the
making of complex decisions. An exciting recent development in the
study of thinking has been the attempt to mimic or simulate human
thought with computer programs.
Imagery. After long neglect, imagery is a bright field in contemporary
psychology. Researchers are studying imagery and creativity, fantasy,
dreams--even the place of television in children's imagination.
Emotion. Why are some people calm, effective, and surefooted
when others are jumpy and scattered? What makes people courageous,
sad, angry, or mean? The answers proposed to such questions have been
various. Some psychologists are interested in the genetics of individual
differences in emotion and personality; other researchers delve into
the nature of emotion itself.
Motivation. Investigators work on the sources of action--motivation--to
determine how much behavior can be accounted for by simple drives
such as hunger, sex, or pain, and how much depends on complex social
motivation such as the need to achieve or the desire to outpoint a
rival.
Personality. No part of psychology is more difficult and contentious
than the study of personality. In recent years the very idea of stable
personal characteristics has come under attack; some researchers have
maintained that people behave more according to the requirements of
the immediate social environment than according to persistent traits
of personality such as good humor or irritability. Many tests of personality
exist.
Abnormal Psychology
The study of abnormal behavior is a branch of personality psychology,
and it is perhaps the kind of psychology that most often finds its
way into the popular press, television, and imagination. Abnormal
psychology includes the diagnosis of mental malfunction, often with
tests of assorted kinds; the systematic description or taxonomy of
abnormal behavior; and the study of the effectiveness of psychological
and pharmacological (drug) therapies.
Social Psychology
When a human being enters into an exchange with another human being,
this interaction is a problem in social psychology. Recent research
has indicated that most people will behave in extraordinary ways--for
example, they will be willing to injure an innocent person if someone
in authority tells them that such behavior is in order.
Developmental Psychology
The "general practitioners" of psychology, developmental
psychologists study all aspects of behavior as it changes from birth
to old age. The work of developmental psychologists borders on the
applied fields of educational and school psychology covered below.
FIELDS OF APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY
The practical, important, and demanding task of applied psychologists
is to combine their own experience with their knowledge of basic psychology
in order to solve everyday human problems. Applied psychologists may
help a schoolchild to learn, an assembly line to move smoothly, a
group of executives to reach a wise decision, an abusing parent to
stop beating a child, or a patient in an institution to experience
some relief from anxiety. The tasks are as diverse as those of basic
psychology, but they are different from those of basic psychology
in at least two related ways. First, the problems faced by the applied
psychologist cannot be postponed until new experiments or new theories
are developed; today's problems in the classroom or clinic must be
addressed today. Second, applied psychologists rarely can choose what
human puzzle they will try to solve.
Educational Psychology
Educational psychologists usually work with teachers and with schoolchildren
in an attempt to devise effective teaching procedures and to design
classroom activities that lead to learning; they also counsel students
about curriculum and personal problems.
Industrial Psychology
Industrial psychology brings together information about perception,
management, and social organization to make industries and businesses
operate more efficiently and humanely. Some industrial psychologists
are specialists in organizational behavior--how best to get people
working together--and others concentrate on job satisfaction, public
relations, incentives for work, and aspects of advertising.
Psychometrics
This field lies between basic and applied psychology. Psychometricians
(the term means "mind-measurer") invent and refine tests
of competence and aptitude; give tests of various kinds to discover
human talent, potentiality, or the need for special training; and
examine verbal skills, aptitude for new jobs, personality characteristics,
or the likelihood of emotional disturbance.
Clinical Psychology
The largest field of applied psychology, clinical psychology diagnoses
mental distress and helps psychologically disturbed people find a
more balanced way of living. Clinical psychology has, of course, affinities
to psychiatry. Much of the therapy of clinical psychologists is carried
out in person-to-person meetings, but, increasingly over the years,
clinical psychologists have worked with larger groups, from two-person
groups such as spouses or parent and child to a dozen or more residents
of an institution.
METHODS PSYCHOLOGISTS USE
Just as the research and practical interests of psychologists are
diverse, so are their methods. Some applied procedures--tests or therapy
methods--have been mentioned earlier. The present section will focus
on research methods.
Random-Assignment Experiment
The surest and most reliable way to find out the sources of behavior
is to conduct an experiment. Two (or more) groups of subjects are
selected randomly, so that there is no bias in their selection, and
then each group is given a different experience. For example, one
group of young sparrows is raised with adult sparrows, a second group
with adult wrens. In this way one may learn something about transmission
of species-specific birdsong. The random-assignment experiment permits
strong inferences from its results. Its use is limited, however, by
the fact that some important human behavior cannot be subjected to
experimental study.
Contrasting Natural Groups
Since the mid-19th century a major method of psychological investigation
has been to observe naturally occurring groups that differ in some
important way. Often the comparison of natural groups is made more
informative by giving the groups a planned common experience. Psychologists
now know, for example, that mentally retarded children in institutions
will not work as hard on their own in a laboratory task as mentally
retarded children (with the same test scores) raised at home.
A problem persists in comparing natural groups: the differences observed
between groups may be related to the defining characteristic (such
as IQ), but they may also be related to some other correlated factor
that defines the groups. Some psychologists feel that serious misinformation
has been written about racial differences in behavior because inadequate
attention was sometimes given to factors other than ancestry--such
as education or economic status--that separates minority groups in
modern society.
Field Observation
Some animal and human action can be studied in its ordinary everyday
setting. A famous group of European ethologists including Konrad LORENZ
made many discoveries about the mating, parental, and fighting behavior
of birds and small mammals. A number of studies have monitored a small
group of children over the first years of their lives (a few for 40
years or longer) in order to understand the development of personality,
intelligence-test performance, or other aspects of individual difference.
Such longitudinal studies of development combine field observations
in hospital, home, and school with laboratory tasks and with intricate
batteries of tests.
The Case Study
Less widely used in research than either the experiment, comparison
study, or field observation, the case study has been a significant
source of new ideas for psychologists. A case study is the intensive
study of a single person or of a few persons; it has been most influential
in three fields of psychology--abnormal psychology, cognitive development,
and language.
Sigmund FREUD's reconstructions of life histories stand as models
for the application of the case study to abnormal psychology: he built
an explanation of his patient's disorders from interviews, free association,
and the analysis of dreams. Jean PIAGET has used the case study in
a different, but equally productive, way: he studied his three children
over the first three years of their lives to outline the course of
early cognitive development. In language studies the case study has
always been important. Case studies are, however, limited in their
generality. Who can say for sure in what ways the person under study
is typical or eccentric? Nevertheless, case studies remain a basic
first step in psychologists' exploration of the human mind.
|